Logo

Logo

Why humans can recognise faces and read?

Ever wondered why humans can read?  A team led by Kolkata-born scientists has found that a special sweet spot in…

Why humans can recognise faces and read?

(Getty Images)

Ever wondered why humans can read? 

A team led by Kolkata-born scientists has found that a special sweet spot in the eye called 'fovea' plays a crucial role in humans being able to focus on computer screens and also read, an ability which is unique to Homo sapiens.

Advertisement

The findings decipher the mechanism that lets humans reading this text, recognising faces, enjoying colours, say the scientists.

Advertisement

Raunak Sinha and Mrinalini Hoon describe themselves as a 'scientist couple' who push the frontiers of neuroscience to better understand vision.

Sinha says this "recent breakthrough in understanding how the most important aspects of our vision works at a cellular level. This work illustrates the physiological basis of how our central vision, mediated by the region in the eye called fovea, works at a cellular level and how it differs in its operation from the region that mediates our peripheral vision".

Vision scientists have uncovered some of the reasons behind the unusual perceptual properties of the eye's fovea.

Among mammals, only humans and other primates have this dimple-like structure in their retinas.

Owls, some other predatory birds, and some reptiles have a similar structure. The fovea is responsible for our visual experiences that are rich in colourful spatial detail.

Figuring out how the fovea functions is essential to the search for strategies to correct central vision loss, including efforts to design visual prosthetics.

"Diseases such as macular degeneration are much more debilitating than deficits in peripheral eyesight because of the importance of the fovea to everyday vision," says Sinha of the Department of Physiology and Biophysics at the University of Washington's, School of Medicine.

The fovea is a specialised region that dominates our visual perception, he explains.

It provides more than half of the input from the eyes to the visual cortex of the brain.

"When you look at a scene an arm's length away," he says, "the fovea subtends a field only about the size of your thumbnail. Our eyes undergo rapid movements to direct the fovea to various parts of the scene." 

The absence of a fovea in most mammals, he says, and technical challenges associated with recording from the primate fovea, led to a paucity of information about how the fovea operates at the level of cellular circuits.

Using advanced techniques, Sinha helped lead a study that revealed that the computational architecture and basic visual processing of the fovea are distinct from other regions of the retina.

The results help explain why central and peripheral vision have different qualities, he says.

Located near the optic nerve, the fovea is at its best for fine tasks like reading. Compared to the peripheral retina, however, the fovea is less able to process rapidly changing visual signals.

This low sensitivity is what makes us see motion in flipbooks and movies. It's also what prevents us from seeing flicker when a computer or TV screen refreshes, unless we glance at the screen (especially the old-fashioned CRT monitors) from the corner of our eye, Sinha explains.

Advertisement